Chapter 9 Part C
Some of the behaviors
that the tribe programs into its young may not be enjoyable to those young
tribe members while they are young or later when they are adults either. Work
is hard. Building shelters is work. Making clothes is work. Gathering food and
preserving it for the winter is work. Raising kids is work. Work is tedious.
But for survival, individual happiness is not what matters. Patterns of living
which maximize the resources of the tribe over many lifetimes are what matter,
and these ways of living do not always make sense to all of the people who are
being programmed to do them. But then tribes that don't teach hard work and loyalty
to family and tribe die out.
To illustrate further, another
example of a moré that seems counter-intuitive to Western minds, but that
works, can be offered here. Polyandry allows and encourages one woman to have
two or more husbands, legally and with the blessings of the community. It seems
counter-intuitive to us. But the practice is not only viable in some cultures,
it even promotes better survival rates. In some areas of the Himalayas, when a
man knows he may have to be away for an extended period, he can pick a good
second husband for his wife. Then he will know that she and the man's children
and property will be protected. If she gets pregnant while he is away, it will
be by someone that he has approved of. (5.)
This train of thought brings us
to a deeper implication embedded in my argument.
Close analysis of human behaviors
reveals that complete explanation of how and why they occur as they do cannot
be built up from a list of individual behaviors and the advantages that
practicing them might give to the tribe that does so. What I am trying to say
here is that we can't reason our way to a moral code for all humans without
first understanding that humans are capable of forming very large patterns of
thinking that we usually call "concepts".
Behaviorism's model of human
thinking is left behind at this point since Behaviorism does not adequately
explain conceptual thinking. It connects stimulus to response in a mechanical
way and then explains some individual behaviors where stimulus and response can
be clearly described in very limited, detailed, and objective terms. "The
organism sees and recognizes these colors, shapes, and sounds, pushes the bar,
and gets the food-pellet reward." I go to work at the big, grey factory,
punch my time card at the clock beside the brown door, put bolts on widgets for
nine hours, punch out, collect my pay, and go home.
But a human can confront
situations that aren’t exactly like, or by sensory evidence even nearly like,
anything that the human has encountered before, and still the human can react
effectively. The English hunter who had never seen a moose, kangaroo, or
rhinoceros, in muskeg, outback, or veldt, still knew where to shoot in order to
kill one. Polynesian sailors navigated quite well by the stars of a new
hemisphere when they first came to Hawaii as did European sailors when they
first began to explore the lands and seas south of the equator. What these people had helping them in each of
those situations was a set of concepts: terms and ideas underlying them that
stand for common patterns found in large numbers of experiences. The heart of
the animal lies at the center-bottom of the ribcage, and a heart shot is fatal
for every animal on this planet.
Even one particular man may react
in one way to a new stimulus in his first trial run and quite differently in
his next one, after he has contemplated the stimulus situation that is being
presented to him for just a bit longer. He sees, hears, or feels a deeper, more
general pattern that he now recognizes, and then, based on concepts stored in
his memory, he plans and executes a more effective response to it. The lists of
concepts and their uses could go on for pages.
Nearly every human past about
eleven or twelve years old is capable of forming generalizations based on what
he has learned from his individual experiences and, even more, what he has been
taught by the adults of his society. Conceptual thinking is as human as having
forty-six chromosomes. It comes to a child at the age when, for example, s/he
realizes that the short, wide cup holds more soda than the tall, slim one. Volume is a concept. (I take Piaget and his successors as my guides
here.) (6.)
The programmers of society
(parents, teachers, shamen, etc.) make use of this faculty in the young
subjects that they are programming, and greatly increase these persons’ chances
of surviving, by programming them with more than simple, one-to-one responses
to sense data patterns that occur in recognizable repetitions in the tribe's
territory. The young subject is ready to be programmed with “categories” and
then, at higher levels of generality, with "principles" and “values”.
Every tribe has labels (words) for large groups (categories) of similar things
or events in the tribe's environment. These category-terms are taught to the
young because they are useful in the quest for survival. The Sami (Laplanders) have many words for describing a reindeer because they sometimes need a short way to refer
to a dark brown, pregnant doe who is pregnant for the first time in order to find
her in a hurry. She is in labor, in distress, and in need of immediate care.
And for Cro-Magnon tribes, it probably
was useful to have many terms for rock or stone or boulder or pebble or flint
because only the flint could be used to make really effective weapons and
tools. On the other hand, visitors speak only of reindeer does, bucks, and fawns, or may have no words for reindeer at all.
"Principles"
are terms for patterns that are common in even larger groups of events.
"Danger" and "edible" are very general in their range of
application, but still very useful in real life. The first allows one tribe
member to tell another to get away from some object or animal or area
immediately. And stay away. The second allows one tribe member to tell another
that the substance they are both looking at is worth gathering and keeping,
even if, sometimes, it doesn't taste or smell very nice.
The society or tribe has gradually learned
that these very general terms are really widely useful in providing guidelines
for the design of patterns of behavior that will be effective in the tribe's struggle
to survive. And at last we come to "values" which are the most
general of principles, ones that apply to huge patterns in our memories of
sense data. We care about defining "good" because, deep down, we
believe we need to know what it is in order to survive in increasing numbers
over the long haul.
Principles
and values, then, name meta-behaviors, programs that are called up and run within
the confines of the human skull. By using principles and values that we have
learned from our elders, we form judgments about what we are seeing all of the
time. Note, however, that we don't always, or even most of the time, take any
action when an experience is evoking one of our principles. Sometimes we just
see something pop into our locale, and we recognize that it's harmless so we
cease to think about it. Being aware of, and wary of, the details in our
surroundings all of the time does not always mean that we are going to be
taking any action, even though we are always contemplating whole arrays of
possible courses of action. Thinking, even thinking about our ways of thinking
and which ways have been getting good results lately, is a kind of internal
behavior. Often, what shows on the outside – to the frustration of the behaviorists
who insist on studying only what is objectively observable – is nothing at all.
Some
ways of thinking enhance our chances of finding health and survival. Those are
the ways that tribes are seeking, constantly. The ways of thinking that seem to work
most effectively over generations are the ones that we keep and then teach to
our kids. On the other hand, people who live by concepts and principles that don’t work in
reality don’t survive and, therefore, don’t have kids themselves. In short,
principles and values can be understood as tested and proven useful thinking
techniques. They help us to organize our sense data and our memories of sense
data, and they help us to formulate effective plans of action for many people over many generations.
Notes
5.http://www.theatlantic.com/health/archive/2013/02/
when-taking-multiple-husbands-makes-sense/272726/
6.http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/
Piaget's_theory_of_cognitive_development
No comments:
Post a Comment
What are your thoughts now? Comment and I will reply. I promise.