Chapter 12 The Mechanism of Cultural Evolution
geisha dancers
(credit: Joi Ito, via Wikimedia Commons)
dabbing
(credit: Gokudabbing, via Wikimedia Commons)
In order to begin to build
a universal moral code, we must now create a model of human cultural evolution.
One that is reasonable and testable, as all theories in Science are supposed to
be. In order to set up such a model, first, we must describe some data, i.e. in
this case, tell in a general way how a number of morés and customs have
operated in the daily life of several different nations. Second, we must extract
from our observations of data, a theory of how the moral codes that underlie all
human tribes work, i.e. what the software behind human tribal behavior looks
like, how parts of it evolve, and how, sometimes, parts fade out. Then, to
complete this part of our argument, we will test the theory.
The testing of our
theory will have to be ex post facto. That is, we could never intentionally
program a new moral code into a test population even of a few hundred people
just to see how, over a dozen generations or so, that code affects their
survival rates. That would be morally forbidden under the code of ethics we in
the West live by now. We can’t purposely, consciously usurp the freedom and
dignity of other people for reasons of research. But we can examine the records
we have of human tribes, their explicitly stated values and beliefs, and the
choices people in those tribes make and have made in real life. In short, we should
be able, through the study of History, to explain why we humans do the things
we do.
Most of us are raised
and conditioned to be fiercely loyal to the way of life we grew up with, so we
can expect that analyzing the roots of morality will be hard. Powerful, subtle
mental programming steers us away from any such analyzing and toward affirming
the morals and morés that we grew up with. But “hard” doesn’t mean “impossible”.
Most importantly, we have a lot of evidence of life as it has been lived by
real people – i.e. History – to check our theory against.
To begin with, we can
observe the everyday actions of the people around us. Why does this man rise
when his alarm clock rings? Why does he even own an alarm clock? Why do men in
some cultures shave off their beards? Why have women in so many cultures for so
long been so unjustly oppressed? Why is honoring elders such a widespread
custom?
In similar ways,
dozens of mundane questions may be posed about everyday life in our society or
any society. While these actions and the motivations behind them may seem
obvious to the people who live in the society where they are practiced, to
people in other cultures, the reasons for foreigners’ ways aren’t so much
confusing as inscrutable. To outsiders, no nations’ ways are normal.
dancers in West Africa
(credit: Eric Draper, via Wikimedia Commons)
An interesting
example of a custom that is commonplace in some societies but not others is the
one that trains men to shave their beards. In some cultures, clean-shaven men
are seen as being presentable, neat, and attractive. Socially acceptable. In
other cultures, a man without a beard is seen as being weak.
The fascinating
questions come when we ask “Why?” Why is shaving done? Is there a survival
advantage in some environments for men who learned from their fathers to shave
off their beards? For example, do men who shave daily appear younger and, thus,
more attractive to women? Do they reproduce more prolifically and thus pass
their shaving behavior on to more progeny, i.e. sons who watch them shave and
so do the same themselves one day?
Research on shaving
is sparse and inconclusive. However, what’s important to see for now is that
asking these kinds of questions about cultural morés and customs in terms of
their possible advantages in the survival game entails thinking scientifically
about morés. Under this view, no human actions are trivial. They all have
significance in the larger design of a culture. Under this view, we also can
compare cultures; then mundane customs become fascinating.
If we keep asking
"Why?" about our "ways of life", the answers seem to spread
further and further from one another into a great variety of human morés and
then whole cultures; human morés vary widely within any given society and then much
more so from society to society. But if we persist in analyzing our observations
of human behavior, patterns begin to emerge. Based on these patterns, we can
make some general statements about people and their ways.
For the most part,
people act in the ways that they do because they have been programmed to act in
those ways – by their parents and their teachers, and then by the media in
their cultures. Humans don’t anywhere come by their “ways” by genetic
programming. We are not born to adopt shaving our beards or speaking English or
cooking our food because of any “innate” forces pushing us to do these things.
They are learned from those around us as we develop in childhood.
For example,
close observation shows that the vast majority of humans early on in their
development learn to urinate and defecate in ways considered socially
acceptable in their particular culture.
Balut (soft-boiled fetal duck, Vietnam)
(credit: Marshall
Astor, via Wikimedia Commons)
In this category of
mundane morés, we also find the morés that govern how we eat. I far prefer
to eat dishes I find familiar, ones I ate during my upbringing. And in my
culture, I wash my hands before eating in order to remove disease-causing germs
I might otherwise ingest with my food if I ate it with dirty hands. I have
never seen these tiny animals, but I have been trained to be wary of them.
Therefore, I take measures to neutralize the danger I believe they pose to my health.
For similar reasons, I try to urinate and defecate only in places deemed
acceptable in my society, no matter how acute my natural urges feel.
It is crucial to note
here the profound way in which human behavior patterns differ from those of
nearly all other animals. A turtle need not ever see another turtle, from
hatching to dying of old age, in order to be turtlish. A turtle would not be
able to perform its genetically-driven reproductive behavior alone each mating
season, but it would at least try to find a mate. The rest of the time, it
would live in ways completely normal for turtles, with all its behaviors being entirely
directed by its body’s genetic code.
Creatures like ants,
crabs, and fish that came early in evolutionary history clearly are more fully
programmed by their genetic codes than higher order ones like cats, dogs, apes,
and humans. But even most large, complex animals learn only fractions of their
behavioral repertoires. Most of their behaviors, in other words, are
genetically programmed. Kittens, in time, will stalk balls and then mice and
birds, even if they are taken from their mothers still blind and helpless.
Puppies are genetically programmed to bury bones.
Humans, by contrast,
if raised by dogs, become doggish, and demonstrate few if any human behaviors.
We humans – unlike turtles, apes, and kittens – learn to be our society’s way
of being human by “enculturation,” i.e. almost entirely from other, older
humans.1,2
Most animal behaviors
are instinctive, programmed into animals genetically, especially in lower-order
animals. As we rise up the scale of complexity, we arrive at humans, in whom
most behaviors are programmed by nurture – by their upbringings, in other
words. The knowledge base that you consult in order to respond real-life
situations is called your culture and it is learned, not innate. Put a
dead fish in the ground with each corn seed that you plant; wear your tuxedo
and black tie to the opera. These are customs, not inherent ways.
Mom teaching daughter to cook
(credit: Sgt. Sinthia
Rosario, via Wikimedia Commons)
But if humans act as
they do mostly because of social programming, then we must ask why or how some
behavior patterns ever became established at all in the earliest human
societies? And why many behaviours possible for humans vanished or never got tried
at all? Why don’t most people on this planet eat holly berries or make their
children into slaves? The answer is clear: we keep the morés that help us to survive;
we drop the ones that don’t.
We keep alive those morés
that keep us alive. That will be our hypothesis.
Behavior patterns get
established in a society and passed on, generation to generation, if they
enable the people who use them to live, as individuals and tribes – to survive,
reproduce, and then program the behaviors into their young. If new morés or behavior patterns are to last, then they must
achieve these results at levels of efficiency at least as high as those the
community knew before its people began to try out the new behavior patterns. When
an old moré no longer serves any of its carrier society’s needs, or in fact is
getting in the way of serving survival needs, then it dies out. This is the
theory around which the model of sociocultural evolution is built.3
Understanding the
process by which a new moré enters into the cultural code of a society is vital
to our understanding the survival of morés themselves. None of the phases in a
society’s adopting a new moré necessarily entails any of the others. A behavior
recently acquired by one person on a trial basis may make that individual
healthier and/or happier, but this does not automatically mean he will
reproduce more prolifically or nurture his kids more effectively or teach his
morés to them more efficiently. Other factors can, and do, intervene.
Many examples can be
cited as evidence to support this model. Some tribes in Indonesia once taught
every member of the community to go into the forest to defecate. The individual
had to dig a hole in the earth, defecate in it, then cover the excrement with
earth before returning to the tribe’s living spaces. The “reason”? Children
were taught to hide their excrement so no shaman could find it and use it to
cast an evil spell on such a careless child or his/her family.4
In the view of most
of us in Western societies, the advantages of the practice lie in the way it
reduces the risk to the community of diseases such as cholera. We know by our
Science that excrement carries microbes. Sometimes deadly ones. Similar
practices are taught to people in Western societies (and described in cultural
codes as early as those found in the Old Testament of the Bible).
Or consider another
of our morés. For centuries, many Europeans drank a lot of tea, hot chocolate, and/or
coffee. These customs rapidly became accepted as “traditional”, even though the
dates of their introductions into Europe can be specified to within less than a
decade. Neither tea nor coffee was a traditional beverage in old European
cultures. Scientific reasons for why consuming them was beneficial to human
health were not known until germ theory was found, but the benefits were felt
by their enthusiastic consumers, nevertheless.
In much of Europe,
local water contained dangerous bacteria. But the water for properly-made tea
is always boiled before the tea is brewed. Boiling kills the pathogens. Tea
drinkers gained a modest, but real, survival advantage over those who did not
like boiled beverages.
While the benefits
were mixed because they were offset by the negative effects of caffeine abuse,
the important thing to see is that these people did not need to know anything
about bacteria in order to arrive over generations, by trial and deadly error,
at a custom that enabled them to survive in greater numbers over the long term.
Tea drinkers died much less often during epidemics. Of course, in China, the
drinking of tea had been looked on as a healthful practice for both the
individual and society for centuries by the time Western cultures arrived at a similar
custom.
(credit: Docteur Cosmos, via
Wikimedia Commons)
Innu
grandmother and granddaughter
(credit: Ansgar
Walk via Wikimedia Commons)
Another example of a
moré that guides our cultures can be found in a different area of life, in the
laws of Moses. One of these instructs followers of the Hebrew, Christian, and
Muslim faiths to “Honour thy father and thy mother, that thy days may be long
in the land that the Lord thy God hath given thee.” (Exodus 20:12) The faithful
are instructed to care for, respect, and consult their parents. Therefore, by a
small logical extension, all citizens of the community should be cared for in
their old age.
Honoring our elders
means consulting with them on all kinds of matters. But why did this custom
have a good survival index?
Before writing was
invented, an old person was a walking encyclopedia to be consulted for useful
information on treatment of diseases and injuries, planting, harvesting, and
preserving food, making and fixing shelters and tools, hunting, gathering, and
much more. Knowledge was passed down the generations by oral means. By honoring
elders, the people of a tribe preserved, and thus had access to, much larger
stores of knowledge than if they had simply abandoned their elderly as soon as
they appeared to be a net drain on the tribe’s resources. An elder’s knowledge
often solved small problems or, sometimes, major crises, for the entire tribe.
Over many generations, societies that respected and valued their elders
gradually outfed, outbred, and outfought their competitors.
Imagine an elder in a
primitive tribe. She might very well have said: “We have to boil the water.
This sickness came once before, when I was seven summers old. Only people who
drank soup and herb tea did not get sick. All who drank the water got sick and
died.” Honoring elders is a tribe-saving policy. It is, every so often, the
difference between life and death for the whole tribe.
Secretary Sibelius, Joplin MO (2011)
(credit: By HHSgov,
via Wikimedia Commons)
It is worth noting
that the fifth commandment in its original wording read, “Honor thy father and
thy mother, that thy days may be long …” and so on. “Thy” days, not
“their” days. At first glance, this seems odd. If I honor my parents, they will
likely enjoy a more peaceful and comfortable old age, but that will not
guarantee anything about my own final years. By then, my parents, even if they
are grateful folk, will most probably be long since dead. At that point, they
can’t do much to reciprocate and so to benefit me.
On closer examination
though, we see there is more here. As we treat our elders with respect in their
last years, consult their opinions on a range of matters, include them in
social functions, and so on, we model for our children behaviors that are
imprinted on them for a lifetime, and they, in turn, will practice these same
behaviors in twenty years or so. They will take care of mom and dad. Dad. Me. The
commandment turns out to be literally true.
Note also that there
is a complex relationship between our morés or patterns of behavior and our
values programming. The common behavior patterns in a culture, patterns that we
call morés, are just ways of acting out in the physical realm
beliefs that are held deep inside each individual’s mental realm, beliefs about
what kinds of behavior are consistent with the programmed person’s moral code,
i.e. her/his code of what acts are right or wrong, appropriate or
inappropriate, sensible or silly. More on these matters as we go along.
Honoring parents
enables an increase in the tribe’s total store of knowledge. Not committing
adultery checks the spread of sexually transmitted diseases. It also increases
the nurturing behaviors of males, as it increases each man’s confidence that he
is the biological father of every child he is being asked to nurture. Not
stealing and not bearing false witness have benefits for the efficiency of the
whole community, in commerce especially.
By this point in our
argument, explaining the benefits of more commandments, traditions, and customs
should be unnecessary. A major fact is becoming clear: a moral belief and the
behaviors attached to it become well established in a tribe if the behaviors
help tribe members who practice them to survive. It is also clear that
individuals usually don’t see the long-term picture of the tribe’s survival.
They just do what they were raised to believe is right.
A retrograde custom in modern
times: child labor, Nepal, 2010
(credit: Krish
Dulal via Wikimedia Commons)
Children may not
enjoy some of the behaviors their elders dictate; they may not enjoy them later
when they are adults. Work is hard. Building shelters is work. Making clothes
is work. Gathering food and preserving it for the winter is work. Raising kids
is work. But for survival, individual happiness is not what matters. Patterns
of living that maximize the resources of the tribe over many generations are
what matter, and these ways of living do not always make sense to the people
being programmed to do them. But tribes that do not teach hard work as a virtue
die out.
To illustrate
further, another example of a custom that seems counterintuitive to Western
minds, but that works in some contexts, can be offered here. Polyandry allows
and encourages one woman to have two or more husbands, legally and with the
blessings of the community. It seems counterintuitive to us in the West. But
the practice is not only viable in some cultures, it even promotes better
survival rates. In some areas of the Himalayas, when a man knows that finding
work may require him to be away for an extended period, he can pick a good
second husband for his wife. Then he will know that she, his children, his
property, and children and property of the other man, will all be protected. If
she becomes pregnant while he is away, it will be by a man he has approved of.5 As
long as all three really are faithful to the marriage, the risks of any of them
getting an STD remain small. More surviving children is the result.
All that has been
said so far in this chapter has been supporting this hypothesis: a concept,
belief, or value and the behaviors that it fosters get well established in a
tribe if the value/belief – with its attached behaviors – improves the odds of
its adherents’ survival.
A side-note is in
order here.
This train of thought
on the long-term purposes that morés serve for human tribes also brings us to
an implication deeply embedded in our argument. Close analysis of individual
human behaviors reveals that some of them can’t be completely explained by our looking
at their long-term advantages for the tribe.
We can’t reason our
way to a moral code for all humans until we accept that humans are capable of
noticing patterns in their environments. Re-occurring patterns. We call our
labels/words for patterns concepts. In short, we have to incorporate into
our argument the idea that humans are capable of conceptual thought, what we
usually call “reason”.
Our ways of life are
not just the results of the forces in physical reality. Rather, human ways of
life are mostly made of our creative responses to stressors in reality. No
model of cultural evolution is going to prove adequate to explain that if the
model does not see humans as thinking beings. Humans think in concepts.
Comparing mental
models of things is what we do when we think. When I think of cats, I
mentally form a concept of “cat”. But I never have a cat inside my head. Or a tree
or a nail or any physical thing. And in reality, there are no trees. There are
living things that exist by photosynthesis that I find it convenient to call
“trees”. But each is an individual living thing. And they differ from each
other. Coniferous, deciduous, bushes, bamboo, banyan, and so on. We make up the
terms we need in order to sort memories of real-world things for purposes
useful to us. But reality contains no trees. On the other hand, I can use the
concept of a tree to escape wolves. Sometimes, I may urgently need that
concept.
Thus, I think with
concepts of things, and I am capable of forming conclusions by a process that
can’t be explained in strictly mechanical ways. The “I” that is most “I” is not
made of cats or trees. But I can think about those things and many others and
reach useful conclusions. That is just human.
We humans act much of
the time in ways that our cultures have programmed us to act, but we also can
figure some situations out for ourselves and try new responses to them. We can
learn on our own. Sometimes, creative individuals even add new concepts that get
accepted into their tribe’s whole culture. They win a lot of others over
to drinking tea or washing their hands before eating. The subculture that the
converts form then out-survives those who don’t accept the new custom, and in a
generation or two, the custom is customary.
Behaviorism’s model
of how humans think is left behind at this point in our argument because it does not take into account
how we think. It pictures stimuli and responses as being connected in a
one-to-one, mechanical way. It then uses mechanical terms to explain individual
human behaviors. But in the real world of real
human beings, this model doesn’t work very well.
The behaviorist
reports that “The organism sees specific colours and shapes (or hears certain
sounds), pushes the bar, and gets the food-pellet reward.” For example, a rat
sees a light go on in its cage, presses the bar it has learned by trial and
error to press, and gets a food pellet. Behaviorists say people do the same: go
to work at the factory, punch a time card at the clock beside the door, put
bolts on widgets for nine hours, punch out, collect their pay, and go home.
This picture of activity, Behaviorists say, portrays how all learning and doing
works for all living things – including humans – all the time.
Bull Moose (credit: Ryan
Hagerty, Wikimedia Commons)
But a human can
confront situations that are not, by sensory evidence, like anything the human
has ever encountered before, and still the human can react effectively. The
English hunter who had never seen a moose, kangaroo, or rhinoceros in muskeg,
outback, or veldt still knew where to shoot to kill one.
Polynesian sailors
navigated well by the stars of a new hemisphere when they first came to Hawaii
as did European sailors when they first began to explore the seas south of the
equator. In each of these situations, they were guided by a set of concepts – ideas
based on patterns found in large numbers of experiences. For example, a mammal’s
heart lies at the bottom of its ribcage, just to the left of center, and a
heart shot is fatal for every animal on this planet.
Furthermore, a man
may react one way to a new stimulus in his first encounter with it and quite
differently in his next encounter, after he has thought about the stimulus situation
for a bit longer. He sees a deeper, more general pattern that he recognizes,
and then, based on concepts stored in his memory, he plans and executes a
better response to it. Behaviorism can’t explain such acts.
Nearly every human
past the age of ten is capable of forming generalizations based on what he has
learned from his individual experiences and, to an even greater degree, what he
has been taught by the adults of his tribe. Conceptual thinking is as human as
having forty-six chromosomes. It comes naturally to a child at about seven
years of age when, for example, he realizes that the short, wide cup holds more
soda than the tall, slim one. Volume is a concept. (I take Piaget as my guide
here.6)
The programmers of
society – parents, teachers, shamans, and others – make use of this faculty in
their young subjects, greatly increasing these children’s chances of surviving
by programming them with more than simple, one-to-one responses to common, recognizable
sense-data patterns in the tribe’s territory. The young subject is programmed
with concepts and then, at higher levels of generality, with principles,
beliefs, and values. These enable that young subject to respond
to, and handle, new situations. Recognize that a new animal or plant can be
used for food. Or recognize that a new animal or plant is harmful. (“This snake
or spider probably has a poisonous bite; many of their kind do.”)
Our capacity to
think, to use concepts, gives us an advantage over other species on this
planet. Thus, this human capacity to think in concepts, combined with our
capacity to communicate, is the main factor that enables cultural evolution.
A thinking individual
can imagine a new way of getting food, chipping flint, or curing a disease,
then tell it to her/his fellows. A few try the new way. If it works, and it’s
not threatening some other moré that is “sacred” for the tribe, the new moré
gets taken up. The tribe evolves. Not by genetic variations, but by cultural
ones, i.e. by acquiring in the whole tribe a new concept that gets good results.
Reindeer with herdsmen
(credit: Detroit
Publishing Co., via Wikimedia Commons)
Every tribe has
labels (words) for large groups of similar things or events in the tribe’s
environment. These category terms are taught to the young because they are useful
in the quest for survival. The Sami (Laplanders) have many words for describing
a reindeer because they sometimes need to differentiate between them. A single
word to describe a blond, pregnant doe is useful if she is in labor and needs
immediate aid. And for Neolithic tribes, it probably was useful to have many
terms for rocks – like “flint” – because only certain types of rocks, i.e. flint,
could be used to make effective weapons and tools.
By contrast, most
visitors to Lapland speak only of reindeer does, bucks, and fawns, and some
visitors may have no words for reindeer at all. Most of us today, compared to
our Neolithic ancestors, know little about types of flint.
The word principle is
a term for patterns that are common in even larger groups of things. Terms
like danger and edible name general
principles that a tribe has spotted in many experiences of many members. Terms
for principles are harder to learn than ones like tiger or fruit,
but worth learning because they are very useful in the real world. The
term danger enables tribe members to tell one another quickly to
get away from something. It covers crocodiles, tigers, snakes, bears, unstable
cliffs, quicksand, poison ivy, bad water, etc. It’s an efficient term so it is
worth learning and keeping. I avoid snakes on principle.
The term edible covers
nuts, berries, maggots, eggs, frogs, fish, lizards, and many more things an
individual may come upon within the tribe’s environment. It enables one tribe
member to tell another that the thing they’re looking at is worth gathering
because it can be safely, nutriciously eaten.
California spiny lobster
(credit: Dr.
Kjaergaard [assumed], via Wikimedia Commons)
“Poverty” is a word for a very
general concept
Migrant Mother (credit: Dorothea Lange, via Wikimedia Commons)
Early tribes
gradually learned that more general terms – if they accurately described larger
classes of things in reality – could be very useful because more general terms
help us to design more accurately and quickly behaviors that will more often be
effective in our struggle to survive.
Thus, finally, by
this process of greater and greater generalizing, we come to values,
the most general of principles; they apply to large stores of memories of sense
data. We are taught to care about defining a term like good because
we want, ultimately, to survive in greater numbers over the long run. So we are
taught to do the right thing.
Terms for values name
meta-behaviors, programs that are called up and run within our brains. We use
values terms learned from our mentors and teachers to form judgments about what
we are seeing all the time. Values enable us to prioritize and thus, they enable
not just actions, but whole ways of life. They enable us to decide, second by
second, about all we see: important or trivial? Hazard or opportunity? Act or
not? Now? Soon? Later? Ever? How?
Note, also that most
of the time we don’t take any action when an experience is making us think
about one of our values. Often, we recognize a thing is trivial, so we cease to
think about it. Being aware of details in our surroundings does not always mean
we respond to them in any way that shows on the outside. Thinking, even
thinking about our ways of thinking and which of them have been getting good
results lately, is internal behavior. To the frustration of the Behaviorists,
who aim to study only what is objectively observable, what shows outwardly when
we are thinking is often nothing at all.
Modern medical
theory handling reality: vaccination
(credit: Andrew
McGalliard, via Wikimedia Commons)
Some ways of thinking
enhance our chances of finding health and survival. Tribes are always seeking
those ways. Ways of thinking that work effectively over generations are the
ones we keep and teach to our kids. Conversely, people who live by principles
and values that don’t work in reality don’t survive and, thus, don’t have descendants.
In short, values can be understood as proven mental devices for sorting sense
data and responding to events in real life.
Values help us to
organize our sense data and memories of sense data. Over generations, they help
tribe members, individually and jointly, to formulate effective plans of action
in timely ways. In modern terms, we say values "inform" our thinking.
Since reality is always changing, our values must evolve also, though as I said
above, it is sometimes only by the pain of famine, plague, or war that we amend
or re-write our values.
So, let’s now
consider more data/evidence: the ways in which early humans probably formed and
used early examples of principles and values. Let’s test the theory further.
Early hunting tribes,
for example, likely taught their young people methods of killing elk, fish,
birds, etc. and the useful general principles that underlay all of the tribe’s
hunting practices. Crush or sever the spine, right where it enters the skull.
Or pierce the heart. Or cut the throat. Study tracks and droppings. If the
tracks are in new snow, or the droppings are still steaming, the animal is
close by. There were many species to hunt and many ways to stalk and kill each
of them. Over time, the "thought full" tribes that understood and
taught general hunting principles thrived and multiplied.
A hunter needed far
too many behaviors in his repertoire for those behaviors to be learned or
called up one at a time, so hunting principles were invented. In nearly all
cases, hunters found it useful to recall general rules about what they’d seen
and been taught about their target game’s habits. Using these principles, the
hunters would try to anticipate what the animal would do in the upcoming
encounter, on this particular day in this terrain. The hunters would then
prepare psychologically for violent, team-coordinated, physical action. If the
hunt was to be successful, they would need physical and mental preparation.
The exact process by
which each kill would be made could not be known in advance, but the hunters
knew that they would need to act with intelligence (in the planning stage),
then skill and courage (in the doing stage). At the most general level,
successful hunting tribes needed to teach the values that we call courage and wisdom to
their young. These values are so widely applicable in real-life experiences far
beyond hunting that they enable us and our young to deal more effectively with
nearly all of reality. They give us better chances of surviving, reproducing,
and passing the same values on to our children. Again, it is worth noting that
the mechanism of human evolution discussed here is not genetic, but
sociocultural-behavioral, and it assumes conceptual thinking.
Early human art that
shows conceptual thinking
(drawings in Magura cave, Bulgaria) (credit:
Nk, via Wikimedia Commons)
Planting and harvesting grain; ancient
Egyptian hieroglyphs
(credit: Norman de Garis Davies and Nina
Davies, via Wikimedia Commons)
Agricultural
societies succeeded hunter-gatherer ones, and values like patience, foresight,
diligence, and perseverance rose in importance. These values enable and complement
the farming way of life. They didn’t replace hunter-gatherer values totally and
immediately, but the farmers’ values and way of life grew until they, in their
multiplying societies, made old hunters’ values mostly obsolete. The new
agricultural way of life was better at making more humans faster. At that time,
population was a necessary ingredient in armies and power.
Ruins of Ur, ancient
Mesopotamian city
(credit: M. Lubinski, via Wikimedia Commons)
When hard grains that
could be stored indefinitely were domesticated, cities formed. They were
efficient places to store a tribe’s food wealth in a defensible, central site. The
progress from stage to stage had many recursions. Nomadic tribes with little
food and much aggression were lurking. Aggressive nomads might even, for a time,
subjugate and exploit city dwellers. Two ways of life tested themselves against
each other. But in the end, the city dwellers won. They had more, and fitter, food,
goods, weapons, workers, and soldiers.
Inside the new
cities, governing bodies with administrative offices became necessary to ensure
fair distribution of the tribe’s food and to organize the tribe’s members in
ways that brought both domestic order and protection from invaders. Following
them came craftsmen and merchants who found a central, protected site with a
large population more conducive to the practice of their arts than their old
rural settings had been.
A potter
in action at a potter’s wheel
(credit: Yann Forget, via Wikimedia
Commons )
Cities and their ways
proved fitter for economic progress than decentralized farm communities or
nomadic tribes. More citizens working in increasingly specialized skilled tasks
meant more and better goods and services available and thus, over time, helped
to further increase the population.
Values shifted toward
making all citizens content to live in densely populated neighbourhoods,
causing the rise of behaviors that encouraged citizens to respect their
neighbours’ property. Don’t covet the things your neighbor has and don’t bear
false witness against him. The Bible says these things exactly. An ancient book
containing a code by which a real tribe lived in ancient times. Today we can
see why this code worked: envy, especially in crowded towns, raises the odds of
citizens slipping into friction and then violence.
The commandments may
please God; we don't know. But for sure we know these commandments make it
easier for people to live together and get along. Thus, by their commandments,
they increased their tribe’s solidarity, wealth, numbers, and power over the
long haul.
The early city’s laws
expanded the farmer’s guidelines for living in thinly populated farming
communities of familiar faces. These laws prescribed more precisely what kinds
of behaviors were acceptable in nearly all activities of city life. Urban
crowding requires civility. Even the word law came to be
associated with reverent feelings (e.g. for Socrates7).
Most of all, the city
had at its immediate beck and call large numbers who could fight off an enemy
attack. Successful cities even progressed to the point where they could afford
to keep, feed, arm, and train full-time soldiers, professionals who were capable
of outfighting any swarm of amateurs. Farmers remaining in the hinterland moved
closer to the city because life was safer there.
One generation of
life in or near the city taught citizens to be patriotic to their new state.
The cultural programming that successfully reproduced itself made loyalty to
one’s city-state automatic; patriotism is conducive to a city-state’s survival.
In short, patriotism is a program that perpetuates itself. Away from their city
and its morés and values, people came to feel that they could not have a fully
human life. Being fully human meant being Theban or Athenian or whatever was
the term people of a culture used to refer to their hometown.
Ancient Egyptian image of carpenters working
(credit: Maler der Grabkammer
der Bildhauer Nebamun und Ipuki)
(Wikimedia Commons)
Literacy, metals,
machines, factories, and computers all brought values shifts to the nations in
which they first arose. When the ways of life they fostered proved more
vigorous than those of competing societies, the values, morés, and behavior
patterns that rose up with the new technologies were adopted by, or forced on,
those other societies. The values shifts usually also led to revolutions,
nonviolent or violent. Societies that persevered in resisting these shifts in
values and behaviors had to create alternate behavior-generating programs
within their own cultures – programs that were equally effective in the
cultural evolution game – or they got overrun.
Further examples of
morés that illustrate this generalization are easy to find. The fact that so
many of the world’s cultures are patriarchal in design, for instance, is worth pondering.
Female humans appear,
in general, to be marginally less capable than males in some areas such as
large muscle strength and coordination, and in spatial and numerical reasoning
ability.8
But the differences
are small compared with differences among members of the same sex and compared
with the differences between males and females in other species. In addition,
they’re differences that exist between mythical beings called the “average man”
and the “average woman.” Real individuals, male and female, vary considerably
from the mean. Some women are bodybuilders, and some are Math geniuses, while
some men are weak and moronic.
Math genius Emmy Noether
(credit:
unknown photographer, Wikimedia Commons)
Furthermore,
objective, scientific analysis reveals that, on average, females are superior
to males in other ways, such as in coordination of the muscles of the hands and
in verbal reasoning skills. That they have not become the majority of surgeons,
lawyers, and political leaders in most of the world’s societies, jobs for which
they seem better suited, is puzzling to say the least. (Women in the West are
finally achieving parity in medicine, a social change long overdue.9)
Why have females been
relegated to positions of lower status and pay in nearly all the world’s
societies? This seems not only unfair, but illogical and inefficient. Aren’t
such tribes wasting human resources? Unfortunately, logic and fairness have not
been the determining factors. Cultural evolution is subtler, it seems.
Actually, logic and
fairness are just values themselves. In other words, like all values, they’re
tentative. They must serve a society’s survival needs in order to become
entrenched in the value code of that society. If they work counter to the needs
of a society’s survival in vital areas, logic and fairness will be superseded
by what the society will come to call a “higher” value. In the case of women,
for centuries, motherhood was a higher value.
Women bear the young,
and a society’s children are its future in the starkest, most final sense.
Women become pregnant due to anatomy and hormones. We are programmed by our
genetics to find sex pleasurable. We seek it without needing instruction. The
biological drive toward sex is often harnessed and redirected by society’s
programming to serve several of society’s needs at the same time, but these need
not concern us for now. Our line of reasoning has to continue to follow the
developing child – society’s future – in the female womb.
Human females, like almost
all mammalian females, are not as capable of running, hiding, gathering, and
fighting when in advanced pregnancy as when they are not pregnant. After
delivery, the child requires years of nurture before it matures and becomes
able to fend for itself and make adult contributions to society. In short, for
thousands of years, if a society was to survive, its females were needed to
raise kids. Males, in smarter societies, were then programmed into a role which
also supported the nurture of children. It is important to note here that a
male was simply more likely to provide assistance and protection when he
believed that the children were his. These needs led to patriarchal
societies programming women for nurturing and submissive roles. Societies
needed females as willing moms who stayed home and obeyed. First their fathers,
then their husbands.
Individual males who
loved all children were not numerous enough to make a difference to the
long-term odds. Those odds were improved most significantly when most of the
men knew (or thought they knew) which kids were biologically theirs. Let me say
again that this cultural design wasn’t fair. But it was effective. Patriarchy
made population and, thus, it made power.
"Made."
Past tense. Today, in post-industrial society, patriarchy is a cultural design
that has become obsolete. It therefore should be allowed to simply go extinct.
We will discuss this point more in coming chapters.
Note also that male
arousal and orgasm are necessary to procreation; female orgasm is not.
Therefore, societies teaching males to be dominant and females to be submissive
thrived, while competing societies that didn’t teach such values did not. The
logical upshot was that nearly all societies that reproduced at a rate that
enabled them to grow taught their girls to be sexually faithful and generally
submissive to their husbands. Hunter-gatherer societies, agricultural
societies, and industrial societies all grew steadily stronger under patriarchy.
In addition, these
societies evolved toward augmenting their belief in female submissiveness with
supporting values and morés that, in most matters, gave the community’s
approval to male dominance. Other less patriarchal societies stagnated or were assimilated
by expanding, land-seizing, patriarchal ones. Whatever increased male
commitment to child nurture raised the tribe’s odds of going on. Again, note
that the history of these societies was often not shaped by a gender-neutral
concept of justice. Justice bowed to nurture. Survival.
In today’s
post-industrial societies with computer technologies (and the changes they have
brought to our concepts of work and home), women can now contribute children
and work other than child nurturing to all areas of their culture’s ongoing
development and life. The imperatives of the past that dictated girls had to
adopt submissive roles to ensure the survival of their tribe and its culture
are evolutionarily obsolete. Advances in birth control technology (e.g. oral
contraceptives) and in child-rearing and nurturing technology (e.g., artificial
insemination, infant-feeding mixtures) have made the chores and joys of child-rearing
possible for men, and for single women, who in earlier eras had little choice
but to forego the joys and trials of parenting or suffer, in themselves and through
their children, cruel social stigma. “Bastard” is an ugly word.
Dad
with infant daughter (credit: Kiefer Wolfowitz, Wikimedia Commons)
In post-industrial
societies, there is no survival-oriented reason for women not having as large
and varied a range of career and lifestyle choices as those open previously
only to men. There is no survival-driven reason for any person not receiving
pay commensurate with the open market value of her/his contribution to the
nation’s ongoing life and development.
Computer programmer
(credit: Joonspoon, via Wikimedia Commons)
In fact, what appears
to be true is that any limitations placed unduly or unequally on the
opportunities of any citizens in the community on the basis of gender, sexual
orientation, or race are only reducing the community’s capacity to grow and
flourish. Computer technology and the oral contraceptive have made a higher
degree of gender-neutral justice possible. If we wish to maximize our human
resources, become as dynamic a society as possible, and compete ever more successfully
in the environments of our planet and perhaps beyond, we must make education
and careers of the highest quality open to all citizens. If we are to maximize
our human resources, then access to education and careers should be based on
merit alone. At least, such is the conclusion we must draw from the reasoning
and evidence we have before us today.
Furthermore, the
authorities of society, if only for efficiency’s sake, probably will have to
find ways of ensuring that quality nurturing of children receives pay and
benefits matching the pay and benefits given to other similar jobs in a society
traditionally driven by these incentives. Having kids will have to be a
reasonable option if we are to maintain a stable base population for our
society in this new century.
Driving women back
into the domestic zone would be counterproductive, like locking our bulldozers
in sheds and digging ditches by hand in order to create jobs. For women and men
who choose it, nurturing children must be given real respect and pay if we are
to continue on the path of knowledge-driven evolution that we have evolved into
and that has now become our way of life.
Whether this
expanding of gender roles and child-rearing practices will endure is still
unclear. Will women be, finally, equal partners with men? Moves toward gender
equity, in work and citizenship, and real change in the everyday life
experiences of women and men, have been tried (to varying degrees) before. And
have faded away before. But the trends in the West, especially at the start of
the twenty-first century, look widespread and strong. The question will be
whether societies that contain a higher degree of gender equity will outperform
those that do not. The answer will emerge gradually over the next century.
This digression on
the sociocultural model of human evolution and examples of familiar morés that
we can imagine being revised is intended to emphasize the fact that our morés
and values are programmable. We can rewrite them by rational discussion and
processes that are based on reasoning, evidence, and compromise. Then, we
put them in the schools in which we instruct our young. For the betterment of
the whole of human society, we can remake us. Difficult, but infinitely
preferable to the blind, inefficient, painful mode of social change that we
have been using for centuries.
It is time for reason
to take over. The hazards of continuing the old ways of prejudice, revolution,
and war are too large. We have to find another way, one that rights gender,
racial, and class injustices without resorting to the horror of war. The goal
of this book is to show that we can find a new way to design our values, a moral
code and way of life founded on our best models of physical reality and the
evidence for those models that lies in experience itself. Then, a
transformation of the nations of the world into one nation will come.
Now let’s return to
our main argument, in spite of digressions that beckon.
Human behaviors and
values almost all originate in the programming put into each individual by his
or her society. In addition, values become established in a society when they
direct that society’s citizens toward patterns of behavior that enable the
citizens to survive, reproduce, and spread in the real world.
It is also worth
noting here that individuals can cause changes to their societies. Changes in
beliefs and customs do not come only from inscrutable processes not accessible
to human detection or analysis. We can see what needs to change and, sometimes,
change it. For most of us, the changes we can bring about are small, but for a Newton,
a Gandhi, or a Martin L. King, those changes can be considerable. But we can
change, we can do it by non-violent means, and brave individuals can be the key
causative agents of that change. There is hope.
We are now able to conclude
this chapter with a major insight into cultural evolution, what it is and how
it works. After looking over many examples of human beliefs and the customs
they foster, we can conclude that the deepest, most general principles that should
guide how we build our values – in big choices for the tribe and small ones for
individuals – should be the most general principles we can spot in the world around us. These are simply the principles underlying our existence, the principles of the universe itself.
If we want to
survive, avoid pain, and enjoy life, first, we have to understand the principles
of the place – this universe – in which we want to do those things.
So, what principles
of reality are relevant to how we build our moral codes? For impatient readers,
I can only say I am coming to them – by small steps and gradual degrees. But we
have to thoroughly discuss the network of ideas at the base of the new moral
system before we try to build the middle and upper levels.
Epistemology first,
then Ontology, then Methodology, then Moral Philosophy.
My proceeding with
care will maximize the chances of my readers seeing that a universal moral code
is possible for us to devise, and that this code of decency and sense, if we
can implement it, offers the only path into the future by which we may survive.
Logically, at this point then, I should discuss more societies of the past,
their worldviews, and how their worldviews shaped their ways of life.
But first, I must
digress for a while. A sad but necessary digression.
Notes
1. http://www.bbc.com/culture/story/20151012-feral-the-children-raised-by-wolves.
2. “Enculturation,” Wikipedia, the Free
Encyclopedia. Accessed April 20, 2015.http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Enculturation.
3. “Sociocultural evolution,” Wikipedia, the
Free Encyclopedia. Accessed April 20, 2015.
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Sociocultural_evolution#Contemporary_discourse_about_sociocultural_evolution.
4. Pearson Higher
Education, “Anthropology and the Study of Culture”
Chapter 1, p. 17.
http://www.pearsonhighered.com/assets/hip/us/hip_us_pearsonhighered/samplechapter/0205949509.pdf.
5. Alice Dreger,
“When Taking Multiple Husbands Makes Sense,” The Atlantic, February
1, 2013.
http://www.theatlantic.com/health/archive/2013/02/when-taking-multiple-husbands-makes-sense/272726/.
6. “Piaget’s theory
of cognitive development,” Wikipedia, the Free Encyclopedia
Accessed April 20, 2015.
http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Piaget’s_theory_of_cognitive_development.
7. Plato, Crito,
Perseus Digital Library. Accessed April 20, 2015.
http://www.perseus.tufts.edu/hopper/text?doc=Perseus%3Atext%3A1999.01.0170%3Atext%3DCrito%3Apage%3D50
8. Mark J. Perry,
“U.S. Male-Female SAT Math Scores: What Accounts for the Gap?” Encyclopedia
Britannica blog, July 1, 2009.
http://www.britannica.com/blogs/2009/07/more-on-the-male-female-sat-math-test-gap/
9. Jenny Hope, “Women
Doctors Will Soon Outnumber Men after Numbers in Medical School Go up
Tenfold,” Daily Mail online, November 30, 2011. http://www.dailymail.co.uk/health/article-2067887/Women-doctors-soon-outnumber-men-numbers-medical-school-fold.html.
No comments:
Post a Comment
What are your thoughts now? Comment and I will reply. I promise.