Normally, testing a new hypothesis involves
performing an experiment that will generate new evidence. If the experiment
delivers new evidence that was predicted by the hypothesis but not by our
background set of concepts, then the hypothesis, as a way of explaining the
real world, seems more likely or probable to us. The new evidence confirms the hypothesis.
But I may also decide to try to use a hypothesis
and the theory or model it is based on to explain some problematic old evidence.
I will be looking at whether the hypothesis and its predictions did in fact fit the old evidence situations. If I find that the hypothesis and the
theory it is based on do successfully explain that problematic old evidence,
what I’m actually confirming is not just the hypothesis and theory but also the
consistency between the evidence, the hypothesis, and my background set of
concepts.
(credit: Wikipedia)
(credit: Wikipedia)
And no, it is not obvious that evidence seen with
my own eyes is 100 percent reliable, not even if I’ve seen a particular
phenomenon repeated many times. Neither my longest-held, most familiar
background concepts nor the sense data I see in everyday experiences
are trusted that much. If they were, then I and anyone who trusts gravity and
light and human anatomy would be unable to watch a good magic show without
having a nervous breakdown. Elephants disappear, men float, and women get sawn
in half. By pure logic, if my most basic concepts were believed at the 100
percent level, then either I would have to gouge my eyes out or go mad. But I
know it’s all a trick of some kind. And I choose, for just the duration of the
show, to suspend my desire to connect all my sense data with my set of
background concepts. It is supposed to be a performance of fun and wonder. If I
did figure out how the trick was done, I would ruin my grandkids’ fun … and my
own.
It‘s important to point out here that the idea
behind H&B is more complex than the equation can
capture. This part of the formula should be read: “If I integrate the
hypothesis into my whole background concept set.” The formula can only attempt
to capture in symbols something that is almost not capturable. This is so
because the point of positing a hypothesis, H, is that it does not fit
neatly into my background set of beliefs. It is built around a new way of
seeing and comprehending reality, and thus it will be integrated into my old background
set of concepts and beliefs only if some of those are removed by careful,
gradual tinkering and then many other concepts also are adjusted.
Similarly, in the term Pr(H/E&B), the E&B is trying to capture something that no math expression can capture. E&B is trying to say: “If I take both the evidence and my set of background beliefs to be 100 percent reliable.” But that way of stating the “E&B” part of the term merely highlights the issue with problematic old evidence. This evidence is problematic because I can’t make it consistent with my set of background concepts and beliefs, no matter how I tinker with them.
Thus, all the whole formula really does is try to
capture the gist of human thinking and learning. It is a useful
approximation, but we can’t become complacent about this formula for the
Bayesian model of human thinking and learning any more than we can become complacent
about any of our concepts. And that last thought is consistent with the spirit of
Bayesianism. It tells us not to become too blindly attached to any of our
concepts; any of them may have to be radically updated and revised at any time.
In short, on closer examination, this criticism of
Bayesianism—which says the Bayesian model can’t explain why we find a fit
between a hypothesis and some problematic old evidence so reassuring—turns out
to be not a fatal criticism, but more of a useful tool, one that we may use to
deepen and broaden our understanding of the Bayesian model of human thinking.
We can hold onto the Bayesian model if we accept that all the concepts, thought
patterns, and patterns of neuron firings in the brain—hypotheses, evidence, and
assumed background concepts—are forming, reforming, aligning, realigning, and
floating in and out of one another all the time, even concepts as basic as the
ones we have about gravity, matter, space, and time.
No comments:
Post a Comment
What are your thoughts now? Comment and I will reply. I promise.