Chapter 6. Part B
Over and over, we act in ways that are not
logical by Bayesian standards. We stake the best of our human and material
resources on ways of behaving that both reasoning and evidence say are not likely
to work. Can Bayesianism account for these glaring bits of evidence that are
inconsistent with its model of human thinking?
The answer to this critique is disturbing.
The problem is not that the Bayesian model doesn't work as an explanation of
human behavior and thinking. The problem is that the Bayesian explanation of
human thinking, and the behaviors that result from that thinking, works too
well. The irrational behaviors which individual humans engage in are not proof
of Bayesianism's inadequacy, but rather of how it applies not only to the thinking,
learning, and behavior of individuals, but sometimes moves up a level to the
thinking, learning, and behavior of whole communities and even whole nations.
Societies evolve, that is learn and change,
in ways that are analogous to the ways in which individuals learn. Like the
curious parts of the individual mind, some curious people in society constantly
examine and test new ideas and new ways of doing things – getting food, raising
kids, fighting off invaders, healing the sick – any of the things that the
society has to do in order to carry on. It is also often the case that other sub-groups
in society view any new idea or way of doing things as threatening to their
most deeply held beliefs. If the adherents of the new idea keep demonstrating that
their idea works, and thus, that the more intransigent group’s old ways are obsolete,
and if that intransigent sub-group steadfastly refuses to re-write its belief
system, then the larger society will usually evolve in a radical way: it will
marginalize or ostracize the less effectual members and their system of ideas.
In this way, a society mirrors what an individual does when he/she finds a
better way of growing onions or or teaching kids or easing Grandpa's arthritic pain.
We adapt, as individuals, but also as societies - to cars, television, vaccinations, email, etc.
But then there are the more disturbing
cases. Sometimes citizens who keep hanging on to old ways that no longer work,
form large minorities or even majorities.
The Bayesian model of human thinking works
well, most of the time, to explain how individuals form and evolve their basic idea
systems. Most of the time, it also can explain how a whole community, tribe, or
nation can grow and change its sets of nation-wide beliefs, thinking styles,
and customs and practices. But can it account for the times when majorities in
the community do not embrace new ways even when the Bayesian calculations and the
evidence show the ideas to be sound? Can the Bayesian model explain tribalism?
Nazi party rally (1938) Tribalism at its worst
As we saw in our last chapter, for the
most part, individuals become willing to drop a set of ideas that seem to be
losing their effectiveness when they also encounter a new set of ideas that
looks more promising. They embrace the new ideas that perform well, i.e. that guide
the individual well, through hazards in real life. Similarly, at the tribal
level, whole societies usually drop paradigms, and the ways of thinking and
living based on those paradigms, when large numbers of citizens keep seeing
that the old ideas are no longer working and that there is a set of new ideas
that is getting better results. Sometimes, on the level of changes that sweep
across a whole society, this mechanism even means societies marginalize or
ostracize sub-cultures that refuse to let go of the old ways.
But when a new
sub-culture with new beliefs and ways keeps getting good results, and the old
sub-culture keeps proving ineffectual by comparison, the majority usually do make
the switch to the new way …of chipping flint, or growing corn, or spearing
fish, or making arrows, or weaving cloth, or building ships, or forging gun
barrels, or dispersing capital to the enterprises with the best growth
potential.
It is also important to state here that, for
most new paradigms and practices, the tests applied to them over the decades only
confirm that the old way is still better. Most new ideas fizzle out. Only
rarely does a superior one come along.
But the more crucial insight is the one
that comes next. Sometimes, if that new paradigm touches on the nation’s most
sensitive central beliefs, then the Bayesian calculations about what individuals
and their society are going to do next break down. When a new idea challenges
those sensitive central beliefs, most citizens continue to blindly adhere to the
old beliefs. The larger question here is whether the Bayesian model of human thinking,
when it is taken up to the level of human social evolution, can account for these
apparently un-Bayesian behaviors.
Many of our most deeply held beliefs are
ones that have to do with those areas of our lives that govern our interactions
with other humans – family members, neighbors, co-workers, fellow-citizens, etc.
These are the areas of our lives which we have long seen, and mostly still see,
as being guided not by reason but by “moral beliefs”, beliefs derived in ways that
are different from our beliefs about the physical world. In anthropological terms,
these are the beliefs that enable the members of the tribe to live together,
work as a team, and get along.
The exploitation of women, the execution
of murderers, and the other anomalies described in earlier posts are merely consequences
of the fact that in spite of our worries about the failures of our moral code
in the last hundred years, much of that code lingers on. In many aspects of our
lives, we are still drifting with the ways that were familiar, even though our
confidence in those ways is eroding around us. We don’t know what else to do.
In the meantime, these traditional ways are so deeply ingrained and familiar as
to seem natural, even automatic, for many people, in spite of evidence to the
contrary.
What we are dealing with when we study the
deepest and most general of these "traditional" behaviors and beliefs
are those beliefs that are very deeply programmed into every child by all of
the tribe’s adult members. These beliefs aren’t subject to the Bayesian models
and laws which usually govern the learning processes of the individual human.
In fact, they are almost always viewed by the individual as the most profoundly
important parts of his culture and himself. They are guarded in the psyche by
layers and layers of emotional associations containing anger and fear. They are
the beliefs and practices that your parents and your teachers and your storytellers
and your leaders enjoined you, with great passion, to hang on to at all cost.
For most people in most societies, these beliefs and their attached morés are viewed
as being simply and truly “normal” and “human”.
Our meta-belief, that is to say our belief
about our moral beliefs, for centuries, was that they were set down by God and,
therefore, were universal and eternal. When we made such a distinction, we were
in effect, placing our moral beliefs in a separate category from the rest, one
meant to guarantee their untouchability.
John Stuart Mill
But are our moral beliefs really
different in some fundamental way from our beliefs in areas like Science,
Athletics, Automotive Mechanics, Farming, or Cooking? The answer is ‘Yes and
no’. Better farming practices and medical procedures we are eager to learn, and
who doesn’t want to win at the track meet? But about the executing of our worst
criminals or the exploitation and subjugation of women, however, many in our
society are more reluctant to change. Historical evidence shows societies can
change in these sensitive areas of their lives, but only grudgingly. (John
Stuart Mill discusses the obstinacy of old ways of thinking about women, for example, in the
introduction to "The Subjection of Women".) (3.)
These beliefs which humans hold most
deeply, the ones that most obstinately resist change in the belief set shared
by a whole nation, are ones that are nearly impossible to amend by rational
persuasion of individuals. They only get eradicated at all if they are
eradicated from a whole nation when they no longer work. They fail to provide effective
real-world guidelines by which the humans who hold them can make choices, act, and
live their lives. They fail so totally in this role that the people who hold the
old values begin to die out. They die young, or fail to reproduce, or fail to
program their values into their young, or the whole tribe may even be overrun. By
one of these mechanisms, a tribe’s whole culture and values system can finally die
out. The genes of the tribe may go on in kids born from the merging of two
tribes – the victors and the vanquished – but one tribe’s set of beliefs, values, and
morés, i.e. its culture, becomes a footnote in history.
Notes
3. http://www.constitution.org/jsm/women.htm
No comments:
Post a Comment
What are your thoughts now? Comment and I will reply. I promise.